|
|
Same stimuli, different responses: a pilot study assessing air pollution visibility impacts on emotional well-being in a controlled environment |
Jianxun Yang1, Qi Gao1, Miaomiao Liu1,2( ), John S. Ji3,4, Jun Bi1,2( ) |
1. State Key Laboratory of Pollution Control and Resource Reuse, School of the Environment, Nanjing University, Nanjing 210023, China 2. Jiangsu Collaborative Innovation Center of Atmospheric Environment and Equipment Technology (CICAEET), Nanjing University of Information Science & Technology, Nanjing 210044, China 3. Vanke School of Public Health, Tsinghua University, Beijing 100084, China 4. Global Health Research Center, Duke Kunshan University, Kunshan 215316, China |
|
|
Abstract ● Emotional responses to visibility-reducing haze was assessed in a controlled lab. ● Valence and arousal have non-linear responses to pollution-caused low visibility. ● Repetitive exposure aggravates negative emotions in severely polluted conditions. ● Emotional bias to pollution relates with gender, decisiveness, attitude to clean air. A growing number of studies have shown that impaired visibility caused by particulate matter pollution influences emotional wellbeing. However, evidence is still scant on how this effect varies across individuals and over repetitive visual exposure in a controlled environment. Herein, we designed a lab-based experiment (41 subjects, 6 blocks) where participants were presented with real-scene images of 12 different PM2.5 concentrations in each block. Emotional valence (negative to positive) and arousal (calm to excited) were self-rated by participants per image, and the response time for each rating was recorded. We find that as pollution level increases from 10 to 260 µg/m3, valence scores decrease, whereas arousal scores decline first and then bounce back, following a U-shaped trend. When air quality deteriorates, individual variability decreases in hedonic valence but increases in arousal. Over blocks, repetitive visual exposure increases valence at a moderate pollution level but aggravates negative emotions in severely polluted conditions (> 150 µg/m3). Finally, we find females, people who are slow in making responses, and those who are highly aroused by clean air tend to express more negative responses (so-called negativity bias) to ambient pollution than their respective counterparts. These results provide deeper insights into individual-level emotional responses to dirty air in a controlled environment. Although the findings in our pilot study should only be directly applied to the conditions assessed herein, we introduce a framework that can be replicated in different regions to assess the impact of air pollution on local emotional wellbeing.
|
Keywords
Air pollution
Emotional wellbeing
Variability
Visual exposure
Emotional bias
|
Corresponding Author(s):
Miaomiao Liu,Jun Bi
|
About author: Tongcan Cui and Yizhe Hou contributed equally to this work. |
Issue Date: 14 September 2022
|
|
1 |
A Baccarelli , F Barretta , C Dou , X Zhang , J P McCracken , A Díaz , P A Bertazzi , J Schwartz , S Wang , L Hou . (2011). Effects of particulate air pollution on blood pressure in a highly exposed population in Beijing, China: a repeated-measure study. Environmental Health, 10( 1): 108
https://doi.org/10.1186/1476-069X-10-108
pmid: 22188661
|
2 |
C Bertram , K Rehdanz . (2015). The role of urban green space for human well-being. Ecological Economics, 120 : 139– 152
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolecon.2015.10.013
|
3 |
M M Bradley , P J Lang . (1994). Measuring emotion: the self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25( 1): 49– 59
https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9
pmid: 7962581
|
4 |
T Burki . (2021). WHO introduces ambitious new air quality guidelines. Lancet, 398( 10306): 1117
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(21)02126-7
pmid: 34563284
|
5 |
A Caplin , M Ghandehari , C Lim , P Glimcher , G Thurston . (2019). Advancing environmental exposure assessment science to benefit society. Nature Communications, 10( 1): 1236
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-09155-4
pmid: 30874557
|
6 |
D A Chapman , A Corner , R Webster , E M Markowitz . (2016). Climate visuals: A mixed methods investigation of public perceptions of climate images in three countries. Global Environmental Change, 41 : 172– 182
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2016.10.003
|
7 |
K Do , H Yu , J Velasquez , M Grell-Brisk , H Smith , C E Ivey . (2021). A data-driven approach for characterizing community scale air pollution exposure disparities in inland Southern California. Journal of Aerosol Science, 152 : 105704
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaerosci.2020.105704
|
8 |
R Dong , R Fisman , Y Wang , N Xu . (2021). Air pollution, affect, and forecasting bias: evidence from Chinese financial analysts. Journal of Financial Economics, 139( 3): 971– 984
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfineco.2019.12.004
|
9 |
S Ferreira , A Akay , F Brereton , J Cuñado , P Martinsson , M Moro , T F Ningal . (2013). Life satisfaction and air quality in Europe. Ecological Economics, 88 : 1– 10
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolecon.2012.12.027
|
10 |
B L Fredrickson , D Kahneman . (1993). Duration neglect in retrospective evaluations of affective episodes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65( 1): 45– 55
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.65.1.45
pmid: 8355141
|
11 |
X Gao , B Coull , X Lin , P Vokonas , A 3rd Spiro , L Hou , J Schwartz , A A Baccarelli . (2021). Short-term air pollution, cognitive performance, and nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drug use in the Veterans Affairs normative aging study. Nature Aging, 1( 5): 430– 437
https://doi.org/10.1038/s43587-021-00060-4
pmid: 34841262
|
12 |
J K Hammitt , Y Zhou . (2006). The economic value of air-pollution-related health risks in China: a contingent valuation study. Environmental and Resource Economics, 33( 3): 399– 423
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10640-005-3606-0
|
13 |
F V Hansstein , F Echegaray . (2018). Exploring motivations behind pollution-mask use in a sample of young adults in urban China. Globalization and Health, 14( 1): 122
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12992-018-0441-y
pmid: 30514342
|
14 |
M Houben , W Van Den Noortgate , P Kuppens . (2015). The relation between short-term emotion dynamics and psychological well-being: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 141( 4): 901– 930
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038822
pmid: 25822133
|
15 |
J L Huang , M Liu , N A Bowling . (2015). Insufficient effort responding: examining an insidious confound in survey data. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100( 3): 828– 845
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038510
pmid: 25495093
|
16 |
N Jacobs, N Roman, R Pless ( 2007). Consistent Temporal Variations in Many Outdoor Scenes, 1– 6
|
17 |
H C Jenkins-Smith , J T Ripberger , C L Silva , D E Carlson , K Gupta , N Carlson , A Ter-Mkrtchyan , R E Dunlap . (2020). Partisan asymmetry in temporal stability of climate change beliefs. Nature Climate Change, 10( 4): 322– 328
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41558-020-0719-y
|
18 |
M Joshanloo ( 2019). Investigating the relationships between subjective well-being and psychological well-being over two decades. Emotion (Washington, DC), 19( 1): 183– 187
https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000414
pmid: 29494203
|
19 |
R M Lal , K Das , Y Fan , K K Barkjohn , N Botchwey , A Ramaswami , A G Russell . (2020). Connecting air quality with emotional well-being and neighborhood infrastructure in a US City. Environmental Health Insights, 14 : 1– 11
https://doi.org/10.1177/1178630220915488
pmid: 32425542
|
20 |
P J Lang , M M Bradley . (2010). Emotion and the motivational brain. Biological Psychology, 84( 3): 437– 450
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.10.007
pmid: 19879918
|
21 |
B Lehman , J Thompson , S Davis , J M Carlson . (2019). Affective images of climate change. Frontiers in Psychology, 10( 960): 1– 10
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00960
pmid: 31156493
|
22 |
A Levinson . (2012). Valuing public goods using happiness data: the case of air quality. Journal of Public Economics, 96( 9–10): 869– 880
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpubeco.2012.06.007
|
23 |
Y Li , D Guan , S Tao , X Wang , K He . (2018). A review of air pollution impact on subjective well-being: survey versus visual psychophysics. Journal of Cleaner Production, 184 : 959– 968
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclepro.2018.02.296
|
24 |
Y Li , D Guan , Y Yu , S Westland , D Wang , J Meng , X Wang , K He , S Tao . (2019). A psychophysical measurement on subjective well-being and air pollution. Nature Communications, 10( 1): 5473
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-13459-w
pmid: 31784564
|
25 |
M Liu , J Bi , J Yang , S Qu , J Wang . (2021). Social media never shake the role of trust building in relieving public risk perception. Journal of Cleaner Production, 282 : 124442
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclepro.2020.124442
|
26 |
M T Marfori, S L Campbell, K Garvey, S McKeown, M Veitch, A J Wheeler, N Borchers-Arriagada, F H Johnston ( 2020). Public health messaging during extreme smoke eve nts: Are we hitting the m ark? Frontiers in Public Health, 8: 465
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2020.00465
pmid: 32984250
|
27 |
T Menz . (2011). Do people habituate to air pollution? Evidence from international life satisfaction data. Ecological Economics, 71 : 211– 219
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolecon.2011.09.012
|
28 |
C J L Murray , A Y Aravkin , P Zheng , C Abbafati , K M Abbas , M Abbasi-Kangevari , F Abd-Allah , A Abdelalim , M Abdollahi , I Abdollahpour , GBD 2019 Risk Factors Collaborators . . et al.. (2020). Global burden of 87 risk factors in 204 countries and territories, 1990–2019: a systematic analysis for the Global burden of disease study 2019. Lancet, 396( 10258): 1223– 1249
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30752-2
pmid: 33069327
|
29 |
M Résibois , J Y Rotgé , P Delaveau , P Kuppens , Mechelen I Van , P Fossati , P Verduyn . (2018). The impact of self-distancing on emotion explosiveness and accumulation: an fMRI study. PLoS One, 13( 11): e0206889
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206889
pmid: 30399153
|
30 |
M G Seo , L F Barrett . (2007). Being emotional during decision making—Good or bad? An empirical investigation. Academy of Management Journal, 50( 4): 923– 940
https://doi.org/10.5465/amj.2007.26279217
pmid: 18449361
|
31 |
H Song , N Schwarz . (2009). If it’s difficult to pronounce, it must be risky. Psychological Science, 20( 2): 135– 138
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02267.x
pmid: 19170941
|
32 |
T Yan , R Tourangeau . (2008). Fast times and easy questions: the effects of age, experience and question complexity on web survey response times. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22( 1): 51– 68
https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1331
|
33 |
J Zhang , Q Mu . (2018). Air pollution and defensive expenditures: evidence from particulate-filtering facemasks. Journal of Environmental Economics and Management, 92 : 517– 536
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jeem.2017.07.006
|
34 |
X Zhang , X Chen , X Zhang . (2018). The impact of exposure to air pollution on cognitive performance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115( 37): 9193– 9197
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1809474115
pmid: 30150383
|
35 |
Z Zhu , Y Zhao . (2021). Severe air pollution and psychological distress in China: the interactive effects of coping and perceived controllability. Frontiers in Psychology, 12 : 601964
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.601964
pmid: 34149499
|
|
Viewed |
|
|
|
Full text
|
|
|
|
|
Abstract
|
|
|
|
|
Cited |
|
|
|
|
|
Shared |
|
|
|
|
|
Discussed |
|
|
|
|